Consciousness From a Neuroscience Perspective

Consciousness From a Neuroscience Perspective
Alejandro Sanfeliciano

Written and verified by the psychologist Alejandro Sanfeliciano.

Last update: 21 December, 2022

Consciousness is a construct that’s not very easy to define. Even though most of us know what it is, there aren’t many definitions that really get to the heart of how complex it is.

If we wanted to define it in an oversimplified way, we’d call it the mental capacity to “actively” know what’s going on around you and inside you. Consciousness is the thing that gives you the knowledge that you are you, and that you’re there to everything happening around you.

When you dig deeply into the human mind, you realize that we have many unconscious processes. For example, no one tells their heart to beat, or tells their tongue how to move when they speak. We just think about doing those things and they happen.

But exactly how unconscious or conscious are all our processes? What characterizes those processes? What are the neuropysiological foundations for consciousness?

lights on in man's brain symbolizing consciousness

Characteristics of conscious and unconscious processes

The first thing a scientist who wants to study consciousness will ask themselves is how they can measure it. The problem here is that we’re talking about something that’s impossible to observe directly.

That’s why we have to take indirect measurements of it. Usually the simplest way to do this is to have someone talk about it. There’s a rule that almost always works: if they can communicate it to you, they’re conscious of it.

At some point researchers also realized that you can send multiple stimuli to your subjects.  Most of the time they’ll talk about some of those in their report, but won’t mention others. On top of that, they also saw how even if subjects aren’t conscious of the stimulus, it still might have an impact on their behavior. 

One example of this are priming techniques. These are basically when you show someone a word and it unconsciously makes it easier or harder for them to recognize a related word later on.

Different levels of consciousness

There are also different levels of consciousness you’ll run into when it comes to cognitive processes:

  • Subliminal processes: when the stimulus is weak or goes away quickly. It never crosses into your conscious mind. But that stimulus might still have an impact on your behavior, or lead to some other kind of process. We should also mention that not all experts agree that this kind of process exists.
  • Pre-conscious processes: when the stimuli are strong enough to cross into your conscious mind. But in this case they still don’t because you aren’t paying attention to them, so they go unnoticed. One example of this is inattentional blindness. It means being blind to very noticeable stimuli because your attention is elsewhere. Here’s an interesting video that will show you how it works.
  • Conscious processes: when the stimulus is strong enough to cross into your conscious mind, and you’re paying attention to it, so it does. In this case you receive the information and then you can actively respond to it.

We should also add that these categories are just levels of the same thing; they’re not stand-alone. That means that any process, at any point, can be somewhere between not perceived and completely conscious. 

The neurophysiological foundations of consciousness

One of the big questions in the study of consciousness is its relationship to biological and neurophysiological processes. Researchers have presented plenty of models about how our conscious processes work, but there are still a lot of questions in the air. Still, the research has identified some of the structures that might play a role, along with why exactly consciousness exists.

The easiest way to study the brain structures related to consciousness is to use neuroimaging tools. That way you can compare conscious and unconscious processes. The results usually tell us that there’s some kind of extra neural activation involved in conscious processes. 

What exactly does that mean?

So, when you switch tasks, the activated areas will change too. That makes it seem like consciousness isn’t centered around any specific structures. It might actually just be something our whole brain does. According to these studies, it looks like the parts of our brain that usually activate are our parietal and frontal lobes. But you still have to take that information with a grain of salt.

Let’s get back to the eternal question: why do we have consciousness? Even though there’s no easy answer, it seems like the most widely-supported theory is that it works like a short circuit. In other words, it’s like a supervising process that evaluates our behavior and “short circuits” any processes where there are mistakes.

This theoretical system would only take action with big processes as a way to save energy and be more efficient. That would also explain why there are different levels of consciousness.

Consciousness is a fascinating, mysterious process that’s kept scientists, philosophers, and neuroscientists guessing for a long time. The more we research, the more we learn about consciousness. But there’s still a lot for us to learn.


This text is provided for informational purposes only and does not replace consultation with a professional. If in doubt, consult your specialist.